Mental Accounting
By Steve Gillman
There is a concept in the science of behavioral economics,
which is referred to as "mental accounting." It is
the process by which we label and treat different sources of
money. The labeling, as the phrase suggests, is largely in our
minds.
For example, we may think carefully before spending hard-earned
money on a frivolous purchase, but if we won the same amount
of money we spend it more easily because we categorize that income
as "found money" or "extra cash." This is
the way we think even though it is the same amount of money and
is just as much ours as any other money we have. Win $10,000
at the casino and it somehow feels different than $10,000 you
saved over the years.
Another classic example used by researchers in this area involves
the difference between "cash" and other categories
of money. For example, researchers will ask people, "Suppose
you bought tickets to the opera for $100, and then lost them
on the way to the event; would you buy another set for another
$100 if you had the money on you?" Most people answer no
when asked this question.
On the other hand, the typical reaction is very different
when people are presented a second scenario: "You are on
your way to the opera and plan to buy the tickets once you get
there. Along the way you lose $100 in the street. However, you
still have enough cash on hand for the tickets, so do you go
to the opera as planned?" Interestingly most people answer
yes to this scenario, even though in both cases the financial
outcome is absolutely identical.
In our first scenario, the person has already spent $100 from
the mental category "opera," so it seems too expensive
to spend another $100 to buy the tickets all over again. But
in the second scenario, the person lost $100 cash, which is a
separate category in his or her mind, making it much easier to
buy the tickets as planned. The two events seem unrelated, after
all. This mental accounting has its consequences, as you can
imagine, and they are often seen as negative, with some degree
of irrationality--or at least non-rationality--ascribed to this
thinking pattern by researchers.
The negative examples are easy to imagine, and one or two
are mentioned above. For example, most people would hesitate
to waste $3,000 that they had saved up by setting aside $30 per
week for two years, but a tax refund of $3,000 is much easier
to fritter away on things that are unimportant to one's goals--even
though it is the same amount and could be used in the same ways.
But the effects of mental accounting are not all negative.
Perhaps you can identify a more positive example of mental accounting
in your own life. Many of us use mental accounting tricks to
keep from spending money, for example. You might think of money
you make from an side job as different than your regular income,
and thus set it aside for a special purpose. You might even reverse
the usual pattern, and see windfalls as an opportunity to save
all of the extra cash to start a business or travel the world.
Mental accounting, then, is something you should be aware
of. Watch to see how it you are doing it now, and choose better
ways to think about money.
|
If you liked this page please let others know with one of
these...
Other Relevant Pages
Behavioral Economics
Money Articles
|